- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Quantitative investigation of memory recall performance of a computational microcircuit model of the hippocampus
Brain Informatics volume 8, Article number: 9 (2021)
Abstract
Memory, the process of encoding, storing, and maintaining information over time to influence future actions, is very important in our lives. Losing it, it comes with a great cost. Deciphering the biophysical mechanisms leading to recall improvement should thus be of outmost importance. In this study, we embarked on the quest to improve computationally the recall performance of a bio-inspired microcircuit model of the mammalian hippocampus, a brain region responsible for the storage and recall of short-term declarative memories. The model consisted of excitatory and inhibitory cells. The cell properties followed closely what is currently known from the experimental neurosciences. Cells’ firing was timed to a theta oscillation paced by two distinct neuronal populations exhibiting highly regular bursting activity, one tightly coupled to the trough and the other to the peak of theta. An excitatory input provided to excitatory cells context and timing information for retrieval of previously stored memory patterns. Inhibition to excitatory cells acted as a non-specific global threshold machine that removed spurious activity during recall. To systematically evaluate the model’s recall performance against stored patterns, pattern overlap, network size, and active cells per pattern, we selectively modulated feedforward and feedback excitatory and inhibitory pathways targeting specific excitatory and inhibitory cells. Of the different model variations (modulated pathways) tested, ‘model 1’ recall quality was excellent across all conditions. ‘Model 2’ recall was the worst. The number of ‘active cells’ representing a memory pattern was the determining factor in improving the model’s recall performance regardless of the number of stored patterns and overlap between them. As ‘active cells per pattern’ decreased, the model’s memory capacity increased, interference effects between stored patterns decreased, and recall quality improved.
1 Introduction
The case of Henry Molaison (the famous ‘HM’ patient) has taught us a great deal of what memory is and what is the cost of losing it [1]. At a very early age, HM experienced intractable epilepsy suffering from minor seizures, which became major in puberty. Later, in life and despite high doses of anticonvulsant medication, his seizures dominated his life, so no longer could work or lead a normal life. In an attempt to cure his seizures, clinicians recommended bilateral medial temporal lobectomy to surgically resect his hippocampi, parahippocampal cortices, entorhinal cortices, piriform cortices, and amygdalae [2]. Although the surgery was a success and HM was able to control his epilepsy, a severe side effect from the surgery gave him a profound anterograde amnesia, an inability to form new episodic or factual long-term memories, although his memories up-to-surgery remained intact. Thanks to HM and other patients [3], we now know how important memory is in our lives and what is the cost of losing it. Without memory, we cannot remember even our most basic experiences, such as what we had for breakfast or where did we park our car, let alone think about the future. Without memory, we cannot learn anything new.
HM has also taught us that the hippocampus is an important brain structure responsible for short-term storage of declarative memories [4]. It is a well-studied brain area from which a wealth of knowledge of cell types and their anatomical, physiological, synaptic, and network properties has been gained [5]. Its principal excitatory neurons are the pyramidal cells (PCs) in regions CA3 and CA1 and granule cells in dentate gyrus (DG). In addition to excitatory cells, hippocampus has a large variety of inhibitory interneurons [6,7,8]. Excitatory and inhibitory cells in the hippocampus form intricate microcircuits, which compute information differently in each hippocampal region. DG microcircuits have been implicated in pattern separation [9,10,11,12], whereas CA3 ones in pattern completion [9, 10] and CA1 ones in novelty detection [13] and mismatch of expectations [14]. These microcircuits exhibit also different rhythms which correlate positively with different behavioral conditions. Theta (4–7 Hz) and gamma (30–100 Hz) oscillations have been shown to co-exist [15] and their co-existence has been hypothesized to support specific functional information processing [16]. Theta oscillations have been implicated in episodic and spatial memory formation [17,18,19,20,21,22] and disruption of them results in behavioral deficits [23].
In 2010, a detailed computational model of the CA1 microcircuit (Fig. 1) was first introduced which showed how memory formation (storage and recall) could be controlled [17]. The model was based on the many details we knew then of the neuronal hippocampal circuit. It showed how theta modulated inhibition separated encoding and retrieval of memories in CA1 into two functionally independent processes and predicted functional roles of various guises (somatic, axonic, and dendritic) of inhibition in these processes. In the model, somatic inhibition allowed generation of dendritic calcium spikes that promoted synaptic long-term potentiation (LTP), while minimizing cell output. Proximal dendritic inhibition controlled cell firing, prevented LTP by suppressing dendritic calcium spikes, and removed interference from spurious memories during recall, whereas distal dendritic inhibition removed interference from new memories been encoded during recall of old memories. The model’s memory capacity and recall performance was tested as more and more memories were stored in its synapses. Results showed that mean recall quality decreased as more memory patterns were loaded into the model’s synapses due to interference with previously stored ones.
In this article, we systematically investigated the biophysical mechanisms that could improve the memory capacity and recall performance of the Cutsuridis et al.’s model [17] by selectively modulating feedforward and feedback excitatory and inhibitory pathways targeting specific excitatory and inhibitory cells in the network model. Our work investigated ways to remove spurious activity and improve the mean recall quality of the network as a function of network size, stored patterns, pattern overlap, and number of active cells.
2 Materials and methods
The complete computational CA1 microcircuit model of Cutsuridis et al. [17] is depicted in Fig. 1. The complete model consisted of 100 PCs, one axo-axonic cell (AAC), two basket cells (BCs), one BSC, and one oriens lacunosum-moleculare (OLM) cell. An entorhinal cortical (EC) excitatory input excited the distal dendrites of PCs, AAC, and BC, whereas an excitatory Schaffer collateral CA3 input excited the proximal dendrites of PCs, AAC, BCs, and BSC. A medial septum (MS) inhibitory input inhibited all inhibitory cells in the network and caused them to fire at specific phases of a theta rhythm.
In this study where only the recall ability of the microcircuit was tested when a growing number of memory patterns were stored in its synapses without examining the exact details of the learning (storage) process, a sub-network of the complete microcircuit model was utilized. The sub-network consisted of N PCs (N = 100 or 300), one BSC, and one OLM cell (see Fig. 2). Simplified morphologies including the soma, apical and basal dendrites and a portion of the axon, were used for each cell type. The biophysical properties of each cell were adapted from cell types reported in the literature, which were extensively validated against experimental data in [24,25,26,27]. BCs and AAC although present in the network were disconnected from it and they were inactive during the retrieval cycle due to strong MS inhibition and hence had no effect on the network dynamics. EC input although present also had no effect on the network cells, because it was also disconnected. The only excitation to the network was from CA3 which excited the dendrites of BSC and PCs. All simulations were performed using NEURON [28] running on a PC with 4 CPUs under Windows 8. Voltage traces of all connected (PCs, BSC, OLM) and disconnected (AAC, BCs) cells to the network with respect to a single theta cycle are depicted in Fig. 3.
To assist the readers of this work and increase the readability of our manuscript, we provide below brief descriptions of each network’s components. Interested readers should refer to [17, 29] studies for more details of the microcircuit model and its components including the dimensions of their cells’ somatic, axonic, and dendritic compartments and distributions of passive and active conductances, synaptic waveforms, and synaptic conductances along these compartments. The complete mathematical formalism of the model can be found in the supplementary online materials document of [29].
2.1 Pyramidal cells
Each PC had 15 compartments. Each compartment contained a calcium pump and buffering mechanisms, calcium activated slow after-hyperpolarized (AHP) and medium AHP K+ currents, a high-voltage activated (HVA) L-type Ca2+ current, an HVA R-type Ca2+ current, a low-voltage activated (LVA) T-type Ca2+ current, an h current, a fast sodium and a delayed rectifier K+ current, a slowly inactivating M-type K+ current, and a fast inactivating A-type K+ current [24, 25].
Each PC received mid-dendritic excitation from Schaffer collaterals (CA3-PCs), proximal excitation from around 1% of other CA1 PCs in the network (recurrent collaterals) [30], spatially distributed (six contacts) proximal dendritic synaptic inhibition from the BSC, and distal synaptic inhibition on each distal [stratum lacunosum-moleculare (SLM)] dendritic branch from the OLM cell.
2.2 Bistratified cell
The BSC had 13 compartments. Each compartment contained a leak conductance, a sodium current, a fast delayed rectifier K+ current, an A-type K+ current, L- and N-type Ca2+ currents, a Ca2+-dependent K+ current, and a Ca2+- and voltage-dependent K+ current [26]. It received excitation from the CA3 Schaffer collaterals in its medial dendritic compartments, excitation from active CA1 PCs in its basal dendrites, and inhibition from MS in its basal dendritic compartments.
2.3 OLM cell
The OLM cell had four compartments. Each compartment had a sodium (Na+) current, a delayed rectifier K+ current, an A-type K+ current, and an h current [27]. It received excitation from the PCs in its basal dendrites and inhibition from MS in the soma.
2.4 Model inputs
An excitatory input originating from CA3 Schaffer collateral pyramidal cell axons and an inhibitory input originating from MS drove the network’s cells during recall (see Fig. 4). The CA3 input was modelled as the firing of M (M = 5, 10 or 20) out of N (N = 100 or 300) CA3 pyramidal cells at an average gamma frequency of 40 Hz (spike trains only modelled and not the explicit cells). PCs and BSC received CA3 excitatory input in their medial dendrites. MS inhibition was modelled as the rhythmic firing of two populations of ten septal cells (MS1 and MS2) each modulated at opposite phases of a theta cycle (180 ° out of phase) [31] (see Fig. 4). Each septal cell output was modelled as bursts of action potentials using a presynaptic spike generator. Each spike train consisted of bursts of action potentials at a mean frequency of 8 Hz for a half-theta cycle (70 ms) followed by a half-theta cycle of silence. Due to 8% noise in the inter-spike intervals, the ten spike trains in each septal population were asynchronous. During recall, MS1 cells inhibited the MS2 cells, which dis-inhibited the BSC and OLM cells in the network.
2.5 Synaptic properties
AMPA, NMDA, GABA-A, and GABA-B synapses were considered. GABA-A were present in somatic and dendritic compartments, whereas GABA-B were present only in medial and distal dendrites. AMPA and NMDA synapses were present only in medial dendrites.
2.6 Network testing
The goal of this research work was to test the recall performance of the model when the network had already stored patterns without examining the exact details of the learning process. To test the recall performance of the model, the methodology described in [17] was adopted. A memory pattern was stored by generating a weight matrix based on a clipped Hebbian learning rule. This weight matrix was used to prespecify the CA3–CA1 PC connection weights. Without loss of generality, the input (CA3) and output (CA1) patterns were assumed to be the same, with each pattern consisting of M (M = 5, 10 or 20) randomly chosen PCs (active cells) out of the population of N (N = 100 or 300) PCs. The N × N (N × N = 100 × 100 or 300 × 300)-dimensional weight matrix was created by setting matrix entry (i, j), wij = 1 if input PC i and output PC j are both active in the same pattern pair; otherwise, weights are 0. Any number of pattern pairs could be stored to create this binary weight matrix. The matrix was applied to our network model by connecting a CA3 input to a CA1 PC with a high AMPA conductance (gAMPA = 1.5 nS) if their connection weight was 1, or with a low conductance (gAMPA = 0.5 nS) if their connection was 0. This approach is supported by experimental evidence favouring two-state synapses [32].
2.7 Memory patterns
Sets of memory patterns at different sizes (1, 5, 10, 20), pattern overlaps (0%, 10%, 20%, 40%), and number of active cells per pattern (5, 10, 20) were created. A 0% overlap between for example five patterns in a set meant no overlap between patterns 1 and 2, 1 and 3, 1 and 4, 1 and 5, 2 and 3, 2 and 4, 2 and 5, 3 and 4, 3 and 5, and 4 and 5. Similarly, a 40% overlap between five patterns in a set meant that 0.4 × Μ cells were shared between patterns 1 and 2, a different 0.4 × Μ cells were shared between patterns 2 and 3, a different 0.4 × Μ cells between patterns 3 and 4, a different 0.4 × Μ cells between patterns 4 and 5, and a different 0.4 × Μ cells between patterns 5 and 1 (see Fig. 5). For 20 active cells per pattern, a maximum of five patterns could be stored by a network of 100 PCs. For ten active cells per pattern, a maximum of ten patterns could be stored, and for five active cells per pattern, a maximum of 20 patterns could be stored. Similar maximum number of patterns could be stored for 10%, 20%, and 40% overlap and 5, 10, and 20 active cells per pattern, respectively.
2.8 Recall performance
To measure the recall performance of our network, the normalized dot product metric was used which measured the distance between the recalled output pattern, A, from the required output pattern, A*:
where NA is the number of output units. Correlation between the recalled and required output patterns took value from 0 (no correlation between output pattern A = [1 0 1 0 1 0] and output pattern A* = [0 1 0 1 0 1]) to 1 (output pattern A = [1 0 1 0 1 0] and output pattern A* = [1 0 1 0 1 0] are identical). The higher the correlation value, the better the recall performance.
2.9 Mean recall quality
We defined mean recall quality of our network model as the mean value of all recall qualities estimated from each pattern presentation when a P number of patterns were already stored in the network:
where Ci is the recall quality of pattern i and Np is total number of recalled patterns. For example, when ten patterns (Np = 10) were initially stored in the network and pattern 1 was presented to the network during recall, then a recall quality value for pattern 1 (C1) was calculated. Repeating this process for each of the other patterns [pattern 2 (C2), pattern 3 (C3), …, pattern 10 (C10)], a recall quality value was calculated. The mean recall quality (MC) of the network was then the mean value of these individual recall qualities.
2.10 Model selection
In [17], BSC inhibition to PC dendrites acted as a global non-specific threshold machine capable of removing spurious activities at the network level during recall. BSC inhibition was held constant as more patterns loaded onto the PC dendritic synapses. The recall quality of the model in [17] decreased as more and more memories were stored (see Fig. 14 in [17]).
To improve the recall performance of [17], we artificially modulated the synaptic strength of selective excitatory and inhibitory pathways to BSC and PC dendrites as more and more patterns were stored in the network:
-
1.
Model 1: strengthening of CA3 feedforward excitatory synaptic drive to BSC dendrites (Fig. 6a) increased BSC’s firing rate. As a result, more IPSPs were generated in the PC dendrites producing a very strong inhibitory environment, which eliminated all spurious activity.
-
2.
Model 2: strengthening of BSC feedforward inhibitory synaptic drive to PC dendrites (Fig. 6b) produced fewer IPSPs, but with greater amplitude.
-
3.
Model 3: strengthening of PC feedback excitatory synaptic drive to BSC basal dendrites (Fig. 6c) had a similar effect as model 1, but with smaller potency.
3 Results
A set of patterns (1, 5, 10, 20) at various percent overlaps (0%, 10%, 20%, 40%) were stored by different number of ‘active cells per pattern’ (5, 10, 20) without recourse to a learning rule by generating a weight matrix based on a clipped Hebbian learning rule, and using the weight matrix to prespecify the CA3 to CA1 PC connection weights. To test recall of a previously stored memory pattern in the model, the entire associated input pattern was applied as a cue in the form of spiking of active CA3 inputs (those belonging to the pattern) distributed within a gamma frequency time window. The cue pattern was repeated at gamma frequency (40 Hz). During the retrieval, only the BSCs and OLM cells were switched on, whereas the AACs and BCs were switched off. The CA3 spiking drove the CA1 PCs plus the BSCs. The EC input, which excited the apical dendrites of PCs, AACs, and BCs, was disconnected during the retrieval.
3.1 Recall quality as a function of pattern overlap
Figure 7 depicts the mean recall quality of all three tested models (‘model 1’, ‘model 2’, ‘model 3’) as a function of percent overlap between stored patterns. Recall quality was best for all three models (‘model 1’, model 2’, ‘model 3’) when overlap was small (up to 10%) regardless of the number of ‘active cells per pattern’ (i.e., the number of cells needed to represent a memory pattern) and patterns stored in the network. For pattern overlaps greater than 10%, recall quality depended solely on the number of ‘active cells’ representing a pattern and it was independent of how many patterns were stored in the network. When five ‘active cells’ were used to represent a memory, then recall quality was best for all three models across all overlaps and irrespective of memory patterns stored. When ten ‘active cells’ were used to represent a memory, the performances of all three models were comparably similar when five or ten patterns were stored and across overlap percentages. When 20 ‘active cells’ were used to represent a memory, then even for just five patterns stored, the recall quality for ‘model 2’ was consistently worst across all overlaps. The performance of ‘model 1’ was consistently best across all condition, whereas the performance of ‘model 3’ was between ‘model 1 and 2’. The performances of ‘model 2’ and ‘model 3’ get worse as overlap increased (from 10 to 40%).
Similar recall performances were observed when the network size increased from 100 to 300 PCs, while keeping all other network components the same (one BSC, six inhibitory synaptic contacts of BSC onto PCs dendrites, one OLM cell) (see Fig. 8). When the network size increased, a performance improvement was evident even at 20% overlap for all three models and across conditions (active cells per pattern, and stored patterns). At larger overlaps as with the smaller network, the determinant factors for excellence in performance were the number of ‘active cells per pattern’, and modulated pathway. ‘Model 1’ (i.e., strengthening of CA3 feedforward excitatory synaptic drive to BSC dendrites) was consistently best across all conditions (active cells per pattern, stored patterns, % overlaps) and against other models (‘model 2’ and ‘model 3’). A direct comparison of the mean recall quality of the small network (100 PCs) against the large network (300 PCs) showed no significant differences across all conditions (see Fig. 9).
3.2 Recall quality as a function of active cells
Figures 10 and 11 depict the mean recall quality of models 1–3 of the small network (100 PCs) against number of ‘active cells per pattern’ for various overlaps (0%, 10%, 20%, and 40%) when five (Fig. 10) and ten (Fig. 11) patterns were stored in the network. When five or ten ‘active cells’ were used to represent a memory, then the recall performances of all three models when number of stored patterns were five or ten were comparable at 0%, 10%, 20%, and 40%, respectively. This meant that the number of patterns stored in the network did not had any effect in its recall quality. When ‘active cells’ were increased (from 10 to 20), then the recall qualities of models 2 and 3 progressively got worse as overlap between patterns increased (from 0 to 40%). ‘Model 1’ recall quality was consistently best (C = 1) across ‘active cells’, stored patterns, and overlap conditions.
Figures 12 and 13 depict the mean recall qualities of models 1–3 of the large network (300 PCs) against number of ‘active cells per pattern’ for various overlaps (0%, 10%, 20%, and 40%) when five (Fig. 12) and ten (Fig. 13) patterns were stored in the network. Comparable recall quality results to the smaller network are evident. As before the number of stored patterns had a minor effect on the recall quality of the network (‘model 1’, ‘model 2’, and ‘model 3’). When ‘active cells per pattern’ were increased (from 10 to 20), then the recall qualities of models 2 and 3 progressively got worse as overlap between patterns increased (from 0 to 40%). As before, ‘model 1’ recall quality was consistently best (C = 1) across ‘active cells per pattern’, stored patterns, and overlap conditions.
Direct comparisons of the mean recall quality of the smaller network (100 PCs) against the larger network (300 PCs) when five (Fig. 14) and ten (Fig. 15) patterns were stored showed no significant differences across all conditions.
4 Discussion
4.1 General model considerations
A biologically realistic microcircuit model of region CA1 of the hippocampus with morphologically simplified neurons was employed [17, 29]. The model [17] demonstrated that encoding and retrieval of memories can be separated into two independent theta half-cycles paced by theta modulated intra- and extra-hippocampal inhibition. The model simulated the timing of different extra- and intra-hippocampal cells types relative to the theta rhythm in anesthetized animals [31, 33, 34]. Out of the possible excitatory and inhibitory pathways affecting the network’s dynamics, we selected in this study to quantitatively modulate the following three pathways: (1) strengthening of the feedforward CA3 excitatory synaptic drive to BSC dendrites (‘model 1’), (2) strengthening of the feedforward BSC inhibitory synaptic drive to PC dendrites (‘model 2’), and (3) strengthening of the feedback PC excitatory drive to BSC basal dendrites (‘model 3’).
4.2 Outstanding questions of memory research our model addressed
A number of outstanding questions of memory research were addressed by our study. What constitutes a memory at the network level? How many ‘active cells’ can accurately represent a memory pattern and how do these ‘active cells’ affect the recall performance of the network? Is the network’s performance affected as more and more memory patterns with greater degrees of overlap are stored in the network? How does an increase in network size affect the network’s recall performance?
Our study showed that a memory pattern at the network level can be represented by a high gamma coordinated activity of a population of cells (called here ‘active cells per pattern’) (see Fig. 4b). We found that the number of ‘active cells’ coding for a memory pattern was a key determinant factor for improving the recall performance of each model tested. Our simulations showed that as the number of ‘active cells’ coding for a memory pattern decreased, then the better the recall performance of the model was regardless of how many patterns were stored and the degree of overlap between them. This was the case, because as fewer active cells represented a memory, even though many patterns were stored in the network, then the network’s memory weight matrix was dominated with fewer ‘1 s’ and more ‘0 s’, and hence, interference between the stored patterns was not so strong to decrease the network’s recall performance. As soon as the number of ‘active cells’ coding for a memory pattern was increased, then the network weight matrix was populated with more ‘1 s’ and fewer ‘0 s’, and hence, interference effects between stored patterns took over and the network’s recall performance decreased. We found the latter finding to be also depended on the chosen model. ‘Model 1’ performance was excellent across all conditions (‘active cells’, ‘stored patterns’, ‘degree of overlap’, ‘network size’), whereas ‘model 2’ performance was the worst and ‘model 3’ performance was somewhere between ‘model 1 and 2’.
Our simulations further showed that the effect of the degree of overlap between stored memory patterns was heavily depended on the modulated pathway (i.e., the model tested). As degree of overlap increased (from 10 to 40%), the performances of ‘model 2’ and ‘model 3’ got worse. This finding was independent of the number of patterns stored in the network. An increase in the network size (from 100 to 300 PCs) had ‘no effect’ on average on the recall performances of the three models tested. This was because although we increased the number of PCs in the network, we kept all other network components (one BSC, one OLM, six BSC inhibitory synaptic contacts onto the PC dendrites) and conditions (‘active cells’, ‘stored patterns’, ‘pattern overlap’) the same. As we mentioned above, in the smaller network (100PCs), its weight matrix was not saturated (i.e., it had fewer ‘1 s’ and more ‘0 s’), so an increase in network size and in network’s weight matrix from 100 × 100 to 300 × 300 dimensions had a very little effect on the network’s performance.
So why was ‘model 1’ performance so consistently better than ‘model 2’ and ‘model 3’ across all conditions? Why the recall performance of ‘model 1’ was always so outstanding even when more and more patterns were stored, less or more ‘active cells per pattern’ were utilized and greater degree of overlap between patterns was used? As we described in Sect. 2.10, ‘model 1’ was the case where CA3 feedforward excitatory drive to BSC was strengthened, causing the BSC’s firing rate to increase. As a result, more IPSPs were generated in the PC dendrites producing a very strong inhibitory environment (‘cloud’), which eliminated all spurious activity (see Fig. 6a). ‘Model 3’ was the case the PC feedback excitatory drive to BSC basal dendrites increased as more and more patterns were stored, causing the BSC’s firing rate to also increase but not as much as in ‘model 1’. As a result, IPSPs were generated in the PC dendrites producing though a less strong inhibitory cloud, which eliminated most spurious activity, but not all (see Fig. 6c). ‘Model 2’ was the case where strengthening of BSC feedforward inhibitory synaptic drive to PC dendrites produced fewer IPSPs on them, but with greater amplitude (see Fig. 6b). In all simulations, ‘model 1’ outperformed ‘model 3’ across all conditions (overlaps and ‘active cells per pattern’). The main reason for such an outstanding performance of ‘model 1’ was because the BSC dendrites were excited by a higher frequency (40 Hz) excitatory drive (100 CA3-PCs), whereas in ‘model 3’, BSC dendrites were excited by a much lower frequency excitatory drive originating from the M ‘active cells’ (CA1-PCs) that represented the pattern (CA1 PCs fired once or twice per retrieval cycle). Since in ‘model 1’, the BSC firing frequency response was higher than in ‘model 3’, then the postsynaptic effect the BSC had on the PC dendrites in ‘model 1’ was higher in frequency and duration (but not in amplitude) than in ‘model 3’ (see Fig. 6a, c). Thus, ‘model 1’ had a better success at removing spurious activities and improving recall quality than ‘model 3’. Since the BSC frequency response in ‘model 2’ was fixed, but its postsynaptic effect (weight) on PC dendrites was strengthened, then the amplitude of the inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (IPSPs) on PC dendrites increased (compared to the IPSP amplitudes in models 1, 3), but their frequency responses were low (lower than in models 1, 3; see Fig. 6b). Each IPSP decayed to almost zero before another IPSP was generated postsynaptically on the PC dendrites.
4.3 Future extensions
Several extensions to the basic idea deserve further consideration. Although in this study, only three excitatory and inhibitory pathways were selected to be modulated to examine their effects on the recall performance of the CA1 microcircuit model, there are several other pathways that can be modulated (e.g., a combination of ‘model 1’ with ‘model 3’, or a combination of ‘model 2’ with ‘model 3’, etc.). Another idea worth pursuing further is to examine quantitatively the effect of the BSC inhibitory cloud on the PC dendrites. What will be the effect on the network’s recall performance if the potency of the postsynaptic inhibitory cloud is decreased or increased? What will be its effect when OLM inhibition is removed from the network? What will be the network’s recall performance when the same memory pattern that is retrieved is also being encoded? What will the effect when a new memory pattern is trying to be encoded when an old one is being retrieved? Under what network conditions (network size, active cells, stored patterns, etc.) will the performance of ‘model 2’ become better? These are some of the questions our research is currently attempting to address.
5 Conclusions
A bio-inspired microcircuit model of region CA1 region of the hippocampus [5] was employed to systematically evaluate its mean recall quality against growing numbers of stored patterns, increased percentages of overlaps, and decreasing numbers of ‘active cells per pattern’. The strengths of three selective excitatory and inhibitory pathways to BSC and PC dendrites were chosen to be strengthened as more and more patterns were stored in the network, which resulted in three different network models, the performances of which were compared against each other. The recall performance of ‘model 1’ was found to be excellent (C = 1) across all conditions, whereas the recall performance of ‘model 2’ was the worst. One of our key findings of our study was that the number of ‘active cells per pattern’ had a massive effect on the recall quality of the network regardless of how many patterns were stored in it. As the number of dedicated cells representing a memory (‘active cells per pattern’) decreased, the memory capacity of the CA1-PC network increased, so interference effects between stored patterns decreased, and mean recall quality increased. An increase in network size (from 100 to 300 PCs) had no effect on the performance of the three models tested. Another key finding of our study was that increased firing frequency response of a presynaptic inhibitory cell (BSC) inhibiting a network of PCs had a better success at removing spurious activity at the network level and thus improving recall quality than an increased synaptic efficacy of a presynaptic inhibitory cell (BSC) on a postsynaptic PC while keeping its presynaptic firing rate fixed.
Availability of data and materials
The source code of the model and data are available upon request to the corresponding author. The mathematically formalism of the model can be found in the supplementary online materials of [29].
References
Corkin S (2002) What’s new with the amnesic patient HM? Nat Rev Neurosci 3(2):153–160
Scoville WB, Milner B (1957) Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 20(1):11–21. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11
Schmolck H, Kensinger EA, Corkin S, Squire L (2002) Semantic knowledge in patient HM and other patients with bilateral medial and lateral temporal lobe lesions. Hippocampus 12(4):520–533. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.10039
Eichenbaum H, Dunchenko P, Wood E, Shapiro M, Tanila H (1999) The hippocampus, memory and place cells: is it spatial memory or a memory of space? Neuron 23:209–226
Cutsuridis V, Graham BP, Cobb S, Vida I (2019) Hippocampal microcircuits: a computational modeller’s resource book, 2nd edn. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-99103-0
Freund TF, Buzsaki G (1996) Interneurons of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 6:347–470
Somogyi P, Klausberger T (2005) Defined types of cortical interneurons structure space and spike timing in the hippocampus. J Physiol 562:9–26
Pelkey KA, Chittajallu R, Craig MT, Tricoire L, Wester JC, McBain CJ (2017) Hippocampal GABAergic inhibitory interneurons. Physiol Rev 97:1619–1747
Marr D (1971) A simple theory of archicortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 262(841):23–81
McNaughton BL, Morris RG (1987) Hippocampal synaptic enhancement and information—storage within a distributed memory system. TINS 10:408–415
Wilson MA, McNaughton BL (1993) Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space. Science 261(5124):1055–1058
Hasselmo ME, Wyble BP (1997) Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function. Behav Brain Res 89:1–34
Vinogradova O (2001) Hippocampus as a comparator: role of the two input and two output systems of the hippocampus in selection and registration of information. Hippocampus 11:578–598
Hasselmo ME, Schnell E (1994) Laminar selectivity of the cholinergic suppression of synaptic transmission in rat hippocampal region CA1: computational modeling and brain slice physiology. J Neurosci 14:3898–3914
Whittington M, Traub R (2003) Inhibitory interneurons and network oscillations in vitro. TINS 26:676–682
Axmacher N, Morman F, Fernandez G, Elger C, Fell J (2006) Memory formation by neuronal synchronization. Brain Res Rev 52:170–182
Cutsuridis V, Cobb S, Graham BP (2010) Encoding and retrieval in the hippocampal CA1 microcircuit model. Hippocampus 20:423–446
Cutsuridis V, Hasselmo M (2012) GABAergic modulation of gating, timing and theta phase precession of hippocampal neuronal activity during theta oscillations. Hippocampus 22:1597–1621
Cutsuridis V, Poirazi P (2015) A computational study on how theta modulated inhibition can account for the long temporal delays in the entorhinal—hippocampal loop. Neurobiol Learn Mem 120:69–83
Cutsuridis V, Wenneckers T (2009) Hippocampus, microcircuits and associative memory. Neural Netw 22(8):1120–1128
Kunec S, Hasselmo ME, Kopell N (2005) Encoding and retrieval in the CA3 region of the hippocampus: a model of theta-phase separation. J Neurophysiol 94:70–82
Hasselmo M, Bodelon C, Wyble B (2002) A proposed function of the hippocampal theta rhythm: separate phases of encoding and retrieval of prior learning. Neural Comput 14:793–817
Winson J (1978) Loss of hippocampal theta rhythm results in spatial memory deficit in the rat. Science 210:160–163
Poirazzi P, Brannon T, Mel BW (2003) Arithmetic of subthreshold synaptic summation in a model CA1 pyramidal cell. Neuron 37:977–987
Poirazzi P, Brannon T, Mel BW (2003) Pyramidal neuron as a 2-layer neural network. Neuron 37:989–999
Santhakumar V, Aradi I, Soltetz I (2005) Role of mossy fiber sprouting and mossy cell loss in hyperexcitability: a network model of the dentate gyrus incorporating cells types and axonal topography. J Neurophysiol 93:437–453
Saraga F, Wu CP, Zhang L, Skinner FK (2003) Active dendrites and spike propagation in multicompartmental models of oriens-lacunosum/moleculare hippocampal interneurons. J Physiol 552:673–689
Hines ML, Carnevale T (1997) The NEURON simulation environment. Neural Comput 9:1179–1209
Cutsuridis V (2019) Improving the recall performance of a brain mimetic microcircuit model. Cogn Comput 11:644–655. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12559-019-09658-8
Amaral D, Lavenex P (2007) Hippocampal neuroanatomy. In: Andersen P, Morris R, Amaral D, Bliss T, O’Keefe J (eds) The hippocampus book. University Press, Oxford, pp 37–114
Borhegyi Z, Varga V, Szilagyi N, Fabo D, Freund TF (2004) Phase segregation of medial septal GABAergic neurons during hippocampal theta activity. J Neurosci 24:8470–8479
Petersen CCH, Malenka RC, Nicoll RA, Hopfield JJ (1998) All-or none potentiation at CA3–CA1 synapses. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:4732–4737
Klausberger T, Magill PJ, Marton LF, David J, Roberts B, Cobden PM, Buzsaki G, Somogyi P (2003) Brain-state- and cell-type-specific firing of hippocampal interneurons in vivo. Nature 421:844–848
Klausberger T, Marton LF, Baude A, Roberts JD, Magill PJ, Somogyi P (2004) Spike timing of dendrite-targeting bistratified cells during hippocampal network oscillations in vivo. Nat Neurosci 7:41–47
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Funding
This research has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement No 778602 ULTRACEPT.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
NA performed all simulations and data analysis; SY provided the funding of this research; VC conceived the study, supervised NA, and wrote the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
This article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals performed by any of the authors.
Consent for publication
We consent for the publication of this article.
Competing interests
Authors declare they has no competing financial interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Andreakos, N., Yue, S. & Cutsuridis, V. Quantitative investigation of memory recall performance of a computational microcircuit model of the hippocampus. Brain Inf. 8, 9 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40708-021-00131-7
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40708-021-00131-7