Skip to main content

Spreading activation in nonverbal memory networks

Abstract

Theories of spreading activation primarily involve semantic memory networks. However, the existence of separate verbal and visuospatial memory networks suggests that spreading activation may also occur in visuospatial memory networks. The purpose of the present investigation was to explore this possibility. Specifically, this study sought to create and describe the design frequency corpus and to determine whether this measure of visuospatial spreading activation was related to right hemisphere functioning and spreading activation in verbal memory networks. We used word frequencies taken from the Controlled Oral Word Association Test and design frequencies taken from the Ruff Figural Fluency Test as measures of verbal and visuospatial spreading activation, respectively. Average word and design frequencies were then correlated with measures of left and right cerebral functioning. The results indicated that a significant relationship exists between performance on a test of right posterior functioning (Block Design) and design frequency. A significant negative relationship also exists between spreading activation in semantic memory networks and design frequency. Based on our findings, the hypotheses were supported. Further research will need to be conducted to examine whether spreading activation exists in visuospatial memory networks as well as the parameters that might modulate this spreading activation, such as the influence of neurotransmitters.

1 Introduction

Collins and Loftus [1] proposed a model of spreading activation in semantic networks stating that specific semantic memories (e.g., raspberries) are organized into a larger network that comprises concepts (e.g., fruit). Semantic memories are then represented as nodes within these conceptual networks, and the semantic nodes (e.g., raspberry and strawberry) within a conceptual network (e.g., fruit) are more strongly interconnected through associative, bi-directional links than are semantic nodes from different conceptual networks (e.g., raspberry and automobile). The strength of the associative, bi-directional links varies between nodes within a conceptual network, with some connections being quite strong (e.g., raspberry and blackberry) and others being relatively weaker (raspberry and orange). Activation of any given node will spread along associative links to other related nodes that also comprise the network, spreading first to the nodes linked to the first node and then to the nodes that are linked to each of these and so forth. The strength of the connections between conceptual nodes is partially determined by production frequency norms or the frequency of use of the links.

Further, the strength of the connectivity between conceptual nodes is likely related to the Hebbian principle that neurons (or neuronal assemblies that comprise the semantic network) that fire together wire together ([2, p. 70]; see also [3, 4]). The speed of spreading activation is determined by the strength of the associative links between the nodes. The extent or spread of activation is partially dependent on the strength of the initial activation of the node, such that greater initial activation will result in greater spread of activation from that node. Hence, greater initial activation will result in a greater spread of activation from that initially activated node to other nodes, including those with weaker direct or indirect connections to the initially activated node. The spreading of activation then decreases over time and/or some intervening activity.

There have been a number of other models of spreading activation and semantic priming proposed in the literature (for review see [5]). These models all share the common feature of addressing the structure of, and the flow of, information within semantic memory. As such, these models all involve verbal memory networks. However, research has indicated that separate verbal and nonverbal or visuospatial memory networks exist and neuropsychological efforts frequently employ double dissociation techniques between these systems. A number of factor analytic studies have been conducted to investigate material-specific memory. Although the results of these investigations have been far from unequivocal, the results of many support the existence of separate verbal and visuospatial memory systems. For instance, separate verbal and visuospatial memory factors for the Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised have been reported in normal individuals [6, 7]. Other studies using a variety of tests of verbal and visuospatial memory within a larger battery of neuropsychological tests have also found separate factors [811].

Differential involvement of the left and right hemispheres in verbal and visuospatial memory has also been reported (see [12]). Whereas verbal memory is associated with left hippocampal volume, visuospatial memory is associated with right hippocampal volume [13], although not all studies find this relationship. Electrical stimulation of the left hippocampus results in impairment in the recognition of words and stimulation of the right hippocampus impairs recognition for faces [14]. Encoding of words is associated with left hippocampal activation and encoding of faces is associated with right hippocampal and amygdala activation [15]. Others have found that left temporal lobe epilepsy patients evidence relative impairment in verbal memory and right temporal lobe epilepsy patients evidence impairment in visuospatial memory [1620]. Graydon et al. [21] found a decline in auditory verbal memory following left unilateral temporal lobectomy and a decline in visuospatial memory following right unilateral lobectomy. Similar findings have been reported by Pillon et al. [22] who demonstrated double dissociation between patients with right or left temporal lobectomies and performance on visuospatial versus auditory verbal memory tasks, respectively.

Given the existence of separate verbal and visuospatial memory networks, the possibility exists that spreading activation may also occur in visuospatial memory networks. This hypothetical network may have comprised the lines, angles, and shapes to which we are exposed and which comprise the objects that are stored in memory. Hence, in so much as a network of verbal memories exists throughout which a spread of activation flows from one conceptual node to another, then a similar spread of activation might exist in a visuospatial memory network. Considerable overlap may exist between verbal and visuospatial memory networks, in as much as the shapes of objects are stored in a visuospatial memory network and these objects are associated with verbal labels that are stored in semantic/conceptual networks. However, the possibility also exists that a visuospatial memory network exists that is separate from the verbal memory network. Establishing the possibility that a visuospatial memory network exists, though, creates the unique challenge of measuring, manipulating, and investigating this hypothetical network.

Semantic priming paradigms, such as lexical decision tasks, have been a primary method of investigating spreading activation in lexical/semantic memory networks. Lexical decision tasks involve presenting word pairs in a successive fashion with the individual being asked to indicate whether the target stimulus is a real word or a nonsense string of letters. Additionally, the target word is sometimes preceded by a semantically related prime word (e.g., cat–tiger) and at other times the target word is preceded by an unrelated prime word (e.g., screwdriver–tiger). Typically, the reaction time for the related pairs is significantly faster than the reaction time for the unrelated pairs. This finding indicates that the related prime word activated the semantic network associated with that word and hence facilitated judgment on whether the target word is a real word or not.

Priming tasks have also been used to investigate implicit memory for visual objects. The priming paradigm used in these investigations involves presenting three-dimensional objects, some of which are structurally possible and some of which are not structurally possible; in that they contain inconsistencies in surface structure that would preclude them from existing as actual three-dimensional objects. Implicit memory is then assessed with a priming task involving the brief presentation of objects that were presented and other objects that have not been previously seen. The task for the participant is to decide if the object presented is possible or impossible. Priming is then indicated by more accurate decisions about objects that have been previously presented as compared to objects that have not. Using this paradigm, Schacter and colleagues have demonstrated no significant differences in priming between older and younger adults, despite older adults having worse explicit memory for the objects [23]. Also, transformations of the size and reflection of the objects do not seem to affect priming for objects, even though these transformations do affect explicit memory for the objects [24]. Patients with impaired explicit memory functioning from cerebrovascular disorders exhibit priming of objects as well [25]. The results of studies conducted by Schacter and colleagues have supported the contention that structural information about objects is represented in an implicit memory system that is separate from the explicit memory system in which object meaning is stored [26].

Although the priming paradigm used by Schacter and colleagues may be used to investigate implicit memory for visual objects, this paradigm is not very amenable for investigating whether spreading activation may exist in visuospatial memory networks. Investigating spreading activation in visuospatial memory networks would require the stimuli in this priming paradigm to have some meaningful relationship, something akin to the cat–tiger relationship used in lexical decision tasks. To investigate spreading activation in semantic memory networks, some of the stimulus words have an association with the target words, coming from the same semantic network. However, the strength of the bi-directional associations within a conceptual network cannot be manipulated when the stimuli comprise nonsense three-dimensional figures. Hence, a method for measuring spreading activation within visuospatial memory networks needs to be developed.

We have used a completely different paradigm to investigate spreading activation in lexical and semantic memory networks. Our approach to measuring spreading activation is based on the responses from the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) and Animal Naming (AN) test. The COWAT requires the individual to generate as many words as possible that begin with a specified letter in 60 s and the AN test requires the individual to name as many animals as possible within 60 s. Measuring the extent of spreading activation involves calculating the average word frequency for the words generated on the tests. Based on the aforementioned Hebbian principle, words that occur more frequently in the English language should have stronger representations and lower thresholds for activation. Further, because of production frequency norms, more frequently used words should have stronger and more numerous connections with other words in the lexical (COWAT) or semantic (AN) network. Conversely, words that occur relatively infrequently should have weaker neural representations and fewer associations with other words. Because spreading activation is associated with the frequency with which words are reported, high frequency words will elicit stronger spreading activation of corresponding neural networks. One of the results of this increase in spreading activation is that neural networks associated with lower frequency words are proposed to be activated. Hence, when computing the average word frequency for the words generated on the COWAT or on the AN test, increasing spreading activation will result in a lower overall average word frequency since a greater number of lower frequency words will be included. Additionally, decreased spreading activation will increase the average word frequency due to the inclusion of more words with higher frequencies. The frequency is determined by how often the words generated are normally used by speakers-readers of English. Support for this use of the COWAT and the AN test as an index of spreading activation is provided by the results of research using lexical decision tasks. Specifically, this research has indicated that the reaction time for identifying high-frequency words is significantly faster than for low-frequency words [2729]. The longer reaction time for lower frequency words suggests that the adequate activation of the nodes that represent lower frequency words requires greater spreading activation. Essentially, greater spreading activation is required to activate words that have lower frequencies, i.e., are further out in the lexical or semantic network.

Using our word frequency paradigm for measuring spreading activation, we have found increased spreading activation in individuals with relatively higher scores on the Beck Depression Inventory—II [30]. Further, patients with Alzheimer’s disease exhibit increased spreading activation in lexical memory networks and decreased spreading activation in semantic memory networks [31, 32]. Patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) were also found to exhibit increased spreading activation in lexical memory networks [33]. We have also used this paradigm to investigate the effects of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (AChEIs) on spreading activation, finding that AChEIs have the effect of reducing spreading activation in lexical memory networks [34]. Finally, we have recently reported a significant relationship between spreading activation and memory functioning. Specifically, our findings indicated that increased spreading activation was associated with better immediate and delayed recall of a word list [31, 32].

Our word frequency paradigm represents a more “top–down” approach to measuring spreading activation by having individuals create their own stimuli and hence may also represent more of a “free” or “natural” flow of spreading activation within lexical/semantic networks. This approach to measuring spreading activation is also not as constrained by experimental methodologies as are lexical decision tasks, for which specific words are chosen by the experimenter. More importantly, for the purpose of this investigation, our use of word frequencies from the COWAT and AN test to measure spreading activation is potentially adaptable for measuring spreading activation in visuospatial memory networks. Specifically, the Ruff Figural Fluency Test (RFFT) [35] is a visuospatial analogue of the COWAT and is generally accepted as a measure of nonverbal fluency. Research has indicated that performance on the RFFT is sensitive to right frontal lobe dysfunction [36]. Additionally, we have previously reported that heightened delta EEG amplitude over the right frontal lobe is associated with reduced performance on the RFFT [37]. The use of the RFFT to investigate spreading activation in visuospatial memory networks may be accomplished in a manner similar to that of the COWAT. Specifically, the paradigm would involve calculating the frequency of each unique design produced on the RFFT, based on a large dataset from administration of the RFFT. Analogous to the COWAT, designs that are produced less frequently should be further out in the visuospatial memory network, being comprised more abstract designs for which one is not as likely to come into contact. Conversely, more frequent designs should be closer in the network and have stronger connections with other designs as these designs include figures that comprise many of the objects that we see on a daily basis, such as a simple straight line. The purposes of this investigation included constructing a design frequency corpus, determining how spreading activation in nonverbal memory networks relates to demographic variables, examining the relationship between design frequency and performance on tests of left and right hemisphere functioning, and examining the relationship between verbal and visuospatial spreading activation.

2 Experiment I

We sought to describe the development of a corpus of design frequencies based on the RFFT and to explore how design frequency is related to a variety of demographic variables (sex, age, education, intelligence) and to neuropsychological tests of left and right hemisphere functioning. The Vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised (WAIS-R) was used as a measure of left hemisphere functioning do to the larger body of scientific findings on this version in comparison with the WAIS-IV. Research has indicated that patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy perform worse on the Vocabulary subtest than patients with right temporal lobe epilepsy [38]. Left temporal lobe brain damage has also been associated with worse performance on the Vocabulary subtest [39]. The Block Design subtest of the WAIS-R was used as a measure thought to be relatively sensitive to right hemisphere functioning, although not exclusive to these brain regions. Research has indicated that right temporal lobe epilepsy patients perform significantly worse on the Block Design subtest than those with left temporal lobe epilepsy [40]. Right parietal lesions have also been associated with impaired performance on the Block Design subtest [41]. We also used the Finger Tapping Test to gain indications of left and right hemisphere functioning, since finger tapping is associated with contralateral activation of the sensorimotor cortex [42, 43]. Finally, the total number of unique designs produced on the RFFT was also used to determine if a relationship exists between the number of designs and the average design frequency. Our primary hypothesis was that a negative relationship would exist between design frequency and measures of right hemisphere functioning and that no relationship would exist between design frequency and measures of left hemisphere functioning.

3 Methods

3.1 Participants

Our sample consisted of 173 individuals (85 women and 88 men) from the original normative sample for the RFFT [44]. The ages of our participants ranged from 16 to 69 years (M = 41.36, SD = 15.39) and the level of education ranged from 9 to 21 years (M = 14.23, SD = 2.36). There were a total of 12 left-handed individuals and 161 right-handed individuals. The participants did not have any history of psychiatric hospitalization, chronic drug abuse, or neurological disorder.

3.2 Apparatus

Block Design The Block Design (BD) subtest of the WAIS-R is a test of visuospatial ability requiring individuals to arrange 9 blocks in a particular pattern that replicates a picture that is shown to them. The blocks have some sides that are all red, some sides that are all white, and some sides that are diagonally divided between red and white. Some of the patterns use a 2 × 2 arrangement and other, more difficult patterns use a 3 × 3 arrangement of the blocks. A total of 9 different designs are presented for the individuals to reproduce. The maximum score is 51, with scores on individual items ranging from 0 to 4 and bonus points awarded for quick, perfect performance. The age-corrected Scaled Score was the variable of interest in this study.

Design frequency corpus The design frequency corpus (DFC) contains a corpus of 1397 different designs that were produced on the RFFT by our sample. Each design produced on the RFFT is represented in the corpus only once and the number of times (i.e., frequency) each design was produced is provided (see below for specific procedure in creating the DFC). The DFC is organized such that the frequencies of the designs produced are available for each of the three different stimulus patterns. Further, the first stimulus pattern is administered over three separate trials, with trials 2 and 3 containing background distracters. Thus, the DFC lists not only the total number of times each design was produced across the three different trials but also the total number of times each design was produced for each separate trial. The variable of interest in this study was the average design frequency for the designs produced

Finger Tapping Test The Finger Tapping Test (FTT) is a test of motor speed that consists of a mechanical counter mounted on a small (10″ × 10″) board and on which a specially designed arm is mounted that measures the number of oscillations of the finger. Participants are instructed to use the finger of their dominant hand (FTD) to tap the arm of the mechanical counter as fast as possible and then repeat this using the nondominant hand (FTND). The test requires five separate trials for each hand, with each trial lasting 10 s. The average of the five trials was the variable used in this study consistent with the preponderance of the literature using this measure.

Ruff Figural Fluency Test The Ruff Figural Fluency Test (RFFT) was administered to assess design fluency and to calculate design frequency. The RFFT [35] is a measure of nonverbal fluency consisting of five individual parts, with each part consisting of a unique stimulus pattern. More specifically, each of the five parts contains a 5 × 7 array of 35 unique stimulus items, with each stimulus item being comprised a 5-dot matrix (see Fig. 1). Additionally, each stimulus sheet is preceded by three sample stimulus items for the participant to complete. The participants were instructed to draw as many unique designs as possible by connecting two or more of the dots within each of the matrices within a time limit of 1-min. The first three trials contain the same stimulus pattern but with different distracters placed in the background. The fourth and fifth trials each contain a different 5-dot matrix. The total number of unique designs produced across the five trials was used in this study.

Fig. 1
figure 1

RFFT stimuli for the five different trials

Vocabulary The Vocabulary test is a subtest of the WAIS-R that requires the individuals to orally define a series of 35 words that are presented in increasing difficulty. Each item is scored 0, 1, or 2 and the age-corrected Scaled Score was the variable of interest in this study.

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—revised The WAIS-R is a measure of intelligence consisting of eleven subtests, including six verbal tests and five nonverbal tests. Performance on the verbal and nonverbal tests yields both a Verbal IQ and a Performance IQ, respectively. A Full Scale IQ is also derived based on overall performance. The Full Scale IQ was the variable of interest in this study.

3.3 Procedure

This study was approved by the Middle Tennessee State University Institutional Review Board and all participants provided written informed consent. The WAIS-R, including the Vocabulary and BD subtests, was administered according to standard procedures, as were all other tests. The development of the DFC involved administering the RFFT test using standard procedures. Subsequently, each different design produced for each of the three stimulus patterns was recorded. Once all of the different designs were recorded, we counted how many times each of the designs was produced, or the design frequency. As mentioned previously, the first three trials of the RFFT contain the same 5-dot matrix. There were instances when designs were produced on one of these trials but then those same designs were not produced on the other trials. Hence, we not only calculated a trial-specific design frequency (i.e., how many times each design was produced on each trial) but also a total design frequency for all of the first three trials combined (i.e., how many times a particular design was produced across all three trials).

4 Results

We first sought to characterize the DFC. As mentioned previously, there were a total of 1397 different designs produced across the three different stimulus patterns (Stimulus Pattern A = 656 designs, Stimulus Pattern B = 328 designs, Stimulus Pattern C = 413 designs). Regarding Pattern A, there were anywhere from 1 to 10 lines used to create the different designs and the frequencies ranged from 1 to 121. Designs for Pattern B used anywhere from 1 to 9 lines and the frequencies ranged from 1 to 99. Finally, the designs for Pattern C used from 1 to 10 lines and the frequencies ranged from 1 to 101. Designs comprised a greater number of lines were generally the more infrequently produced designs. Please consult Table 1 for specific information regarding the number of different designs produced for each stimulus pattern.

Table 1 Lineage and number of different designs produced for each pattern of the RFFT

We then used the DFC to calculate the average design frequency for each participant. Specifically, for each individual participant, we obtained the frequency for each design produced, using the trial specific design frequencies. The average for each of the five trials was then calculated and then an overall average was calculated from these averages. Hence, an overall average design frequency score was calculated for each of the participants based on the designs they produced. We then conducted correlations between each of the demographic variables of interest and the average design frequency. The results indicated no significant correlations between average design frequency and age (r = .09, p = .124), education (r = −.01, p = .460), or Full Scale IQ (r = −.08, p = .160). We also wanted to know if there were sex differences in regard to average design frequency. The results of a between subjects ANOVA indicated no significant difference, F(1, 171) = .88, p = .348, between the average design frequency for men (M = 40.16, SD = 10.55) and for women (M = 38.59, SD = 11.50). Finally, we conducted a series of correlations between performance on each of the neuropsychological tests and average design frequency. The results indicated a significant negative correlation between BD and average design frequency (r = −.15, p = .028, R 2 = .02; see Fig. 2). No significant correlations were found between average design frequency and Vocabulary (r = −.09, p = .122), FTD (r = −.08, p = .138), FTND (r = −.11, p = .082), and the total number of unique designs produced on the RFFT (r = −.003, p = .485).

Fig. 2
figure 2

The relationship between Block Design performance and visuospatial spreading activation

5 Discussion

The results of Experiment I indicated that average design frequency was not significantly related to age, education, or intelligence. Hence, these variables likely do not represent significant confounds that would need to be controlled in subsequent investigations. Further, we felt that since no relationship existed between these variables, there was no need to stratify design frequency by age, education, or intelligence. Importantly, there was also no significant relationship found between the total number of unique designs produced on the RFFT and the average design frequency. This latter finding indicates that fluency and design frequency are measuring different constructs. The findings also indicated that design frequency were related to a test purported to be relatively sensitive to right posterior cerebral functioning and relatively insensitive as a measure of left hemisphere functioning.

These results suggest that design frequency is a separate construct than design fluency and that design frequency is possibly related to right posterior cerebral functioning. However, the results do not necessarily support the proposition that design frequency is a measure of visuospatial spreading activation. As mentioned previously, we have proposed using word frequencies obtained from measures of verbal fluency as a measure of spreading activation in lexical and semantic memory networks. Using the same reasoning, we have proposed that design frequency obtained from a measure of design fluency might then be used as a measure of spreading activation in visuospatial memory networks. Support for using design frequency as a measure of spreading activation might be obtained by examining how design frequency is related to word frequency. Hence, we conducted another study to examine this potential relationship.

6 Experiment II

We sought to examine the relationship between verbal spreading activation and visuospatial spreading activation by conducting correlations between design frequency and both word frequencies from the COWAT and from the AN test. Additional exploratory analyses were conducted to examine any potential relationships between design frequency and indices of verbal and nonverbal fluency as well as depression. Our hypothesis was that a significant relationship would exist between design frequency and word frequency, since both are measures of spreading activation, but that the relationship would be relatively weak since one is a measure of visuospatial spreading activation, whereas the other is a measure of verbal spreading activation.

7 Methods

7.1 Participants

The sample consisted of 41 undergraduate students (8 men and 33 women) with an age range 18–37 years (M = 20.37, SD = 3.41). There were 4 left-handed and 27 right-handed participants. The participants did not have any history of psychiatric hospitalization, chronic drug abuse, or neurological disorder. They were also not taking any psychotropic medications.

7.2 Apparatus

Animal Naming The Animal Naming test (AN) test requires the subject to name as many different animals as possible, with no restrictions based on beginning letter or any other characteristic. They are permitted 60 s to generate as many names of animals as possible. The variable of interest included the total number of animal names produced.

Beck Depression Inventory—II. The Beck Depression Inventory—II (BDI-II; [45]) is a 21-item self-report questionnaire used for measuring the severity of depression. The items of the BDI-II address problems related to numerous psychological, cognitive, and physiological symptoms. Each item is rated by the patient on a scale of 0–3, with a range of possible scores from 0 to 63. The raw score was used as the variable of interest.

Controlled Oral Word Association Test The Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) requires the subject to name as many words as possible that begin with a specified letter (F, A, and S) within 60 s. However, they cannot use proper nouns, they cannot count, and they cannot use a stem word and then simply add different endings. The total number of words produced was the variable used.

Ruff Figural Fluency Test See description in Experiment I.

7.3 Procedure

This study was approved by the Middle Tennessee State University Institutional Review Board and all participants provided written informed consent. The AN, COWAT, RFFT, and BDI-II were all administered using standard procedures. Following administration of these tests, the frequency of each word generated on the COWAT and the AN test was obtained. We used the Francis–Kucera corpus [46] to obtain the word frequency, recording the frequency for each word generated by the participants. The Francis–Kucera corpus was based on over 1 million graphic words from numerous different sources, including periodicals, novels, newspapers, technical writings, philosophical essays, and writings of fiction. The word frequency for words that violated the rules of the COWAT was not included in the analyses. The average word frequency for the COWAT was calculated by first averaging the word frequencies for each letter used. The average word frequency across the three letters was then calculated, based on the average obtained for each letter. The average word frequency for the AN test was calculated by obtaining the word frequency for each animal named and then averaging across all the names of animals. The average design frequency was then calculated for each participant. Specifically, the frequency of each design produced was obtained and then an average design frequency was calculated for each of the five trials of the RFFT. An overall average was then calculated by averaging across the averages for the five trials.

8 Results

Initial correlations were conducted to determine if any significant relationships existed between average design frequency and performance on the COWAT, AN, and BDI-II. We also wanted to determine whether a significant relationship existed between the number of different designs produced on the RFFT and the average design frequency. The results indicated no significant correlation between average design frequency and performance on the COWAT (r = .04, p = .401), AN (r = .003, p = .493), or the BDI-II (r = −.05, p = .377). The relationship between the total number of designs produced on the RFFT and design frequency was also not significant (r = .19, p = .121). To evaluate our hypothesis, we then conducted correlations between the average design frequency and the average word frequencies from the COWAT and AN test. The results indicated no significant correlation between average design frequency and average word frequency from the COWAT (r = .07, p = .327). However, a significant negative correlation was found between average design frequency and average word frequency from the AN test (r = −.27, p = .04, R 2 = .07; see Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

The relationship between verbal and visuospatial spreading activation

8.1 General discussion

The focus of our first experiment was to describe the creation of the DFC and to determine if our measure of design frequency was significantly related to various demographic variables. The results indicated that design frequency was not related to age, education, intelligence, or sex. We also wanted to determine how design frequency (i.e., visuospatial spreading activation) relates to measures of left and right hemisphere functioning. The findings supported our hypothesis, indicating that design frequency was related to a measure of right posterior functioning (Block Design) but not to a measure frequently found to be relatively sensitive to left posterior functioning (Vocabulary) or to a measure of left frontal (right hand FTT) or right frontal (left hand FTT) functioning. As mentioned previously, evidence exists that supports material-specific memory. Whereas verbal memory has been found to activate the left temporal lobe, nonverbal memory activates the right temporal lobe [47]. Golby et al. [48] reported activation within the left inferior prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe during verbal encoding but right inferior prefrontal cortex and right medial temporal lobe activation for pattern encoding. Encoding of words is associated with left dorsal frontal activation and encoding of unfamiliar faces is associated with right dorsal frontal activation [49]. Double dissociation for verbal and nonverbal material has also been demonstrated following left and right thalamic dysfunction [50, 51]. The findings of right hemisphere involvement in visuospatial memory are consistent with the significant correlation observed in our study between design frequency and performance on the Block Design test since research has indicated that performance on this test is related to right posterior functioning [40, 41] and this also supports the proposition that design frequency is related to right posterior cerebral functioning. However, our findings should be considered preliminary in nature. Future research will need to be conducted to provide further evidence for an association between design frequency and right posterior functioning. This evidentiary support might be obtained by correlating design frequency with additional neuropsychological tests purportedly related to right hemisphere functioning. Functional imaging studies might also be used to provide such support.

Although the significant correlation between design frequency and Block Design test performance provides initial support for right hemisphere involvement in design frequency this finding does not necessarily support the proposition that design frequency is a measure of spreading activation in visuospatial memory networks. To support design frequency as a measure of spreading activation, we conducted the second study which involved correlating word frequency, our measure of spreading activation in lexical and semantic memory networks, with design frequency. Our findings supported the hypothesis, indicating the existence of a significant, albeit modest, relationship between average word frequency from the AN test and design frequency. Furthermore, the relationship was negative, suggesting that as spreading activation in visuospatial networks increased spreading activation in semantic memory networks decreased. The existence of a negative relationship between verbal and nonverbal spreading activation is consistent with models of interhemispheric balance. Specifically, Tucker [52] proposed that the left and right cerebral hemispheres exist in a reciprocally balanced relationship, with each hemisphere opposing and complementing the other. This balanced relationship is mediated in part through the corpus callosum, which is involved in both interhemispheric excitation and inhibition [53]. Thus, increased activity in a region of one hemisphere may generate deactivation or decreased activity in the homologous region of the contralateral hemisphere. Research has indicated that semantic fluency tasks, such as performance on the AN test, are associated with left temporal lobe functioning [54, 55]. Given Tucker’s ([52]; see [56]) interhemispheric balance model and the findings of left temporal involvement in semantic fluency, it might be expected that spreading activation within the semantic networks of the left posterior region would be negatively correlated with spreading activation within the visuospatial memory networks of the right posterior region. The integration of these research findings might also explain the lack of a relationship between average word frequency on the COWAT and design frequency since performance on the COWAT is strongly related to left frontal lobe functioning [55, 5759].

The negative correlation between average AN word frequency and design frequency provides initial support for design frequency being a measure of spreading activation in visuospatial memory networks. However, further research needs to be conducted to provide additional evidence, such as by potentially combining our measure of design frequency with a priming paradigm. Research using lexical decision tasks has indicated that the reaction time for identifying words with higher frequencies is significantly faster than the reaction time for words with lower frequencies [2729]. The longer reaction time for lower frequency words might suggest that activation of the nodes that represent lower frequency words requires greater spreading activation. Essentially, greater spreading activation is required to activate words that have lower frequencies, i.e., are further out in the lexical/semantic network.

A similar paradigm might be used with design frequency by having participants complete the RFFT and then presenting them with a design-decision task. The stimuli in the task could be comprised designs that they had produced on the test in addition to designs they had not produced. Additionally, the frequency of the designs could be varied such that they are presented with designs that they produced with lower frequencies as well as designs they had produced with higher frequencies. The task would then involve having the participants decide whether or not they had produced designs that are individually presented to them and measuring reaction time. Designs they had previously produced should be associated with quicker reaction times, which would support design frequency as a measure of implicit memory. Additionally, analyses could focus on the effect of design frequency. Specifically, the reaction times should be faster for designs that have higher frequencies as compared to those with lower frequencies, thereby providing support for design frequency as a measure of spreading activation.

Further research might also explore the potential relationship between design frequency, i.e., spreading activation in nonverbal memory networks, and measures of visuospatial episodic memory. Increased spreading activation may facilitate recall of information due to the increased number of retrieval pathways or cues from which the information may be accessed [60]. We conducted a study examining the relationship between performance on a test of supra-span list learning and spreading activation in lexical memory networks as measured by average word frequency of the words produced on the “F” trial of the COWAT. Our findings indicated a significant negative correlation between average word frequency and both immediate and delayed free recall [31, 32]. Hence, as spreading activation increased recall of the word list improved. A similar study could be conducted by examining the relationship between design frequency and performance on measures of nonverbal or visuospatial memory, such as the Rey Complex Figure [61] or the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test—revised [62].

Collins and Loftus [1] proposed that activation within conceptual/semantic networks will decrease over time or with some intervening activity. The mechanisms modulating spreading activation through activation or deactivation of these networks are not completely understood. However, neurotransmitter systems may have a role in modulating spreading activation. For instance, dopamine may act not only as a neurotransmitter but also as a neuromodulator [63], modulating the “signal-to-noise” ratio [64]. Indeed, Kischka et al. [65] reported that dopamine increases the signal-to-noise ratio in semantic networks and results in reduced spreading activation. We have found that Parkinson’s disease, which is associated with a depletion of dopamine, is associated with increased spreading activation in lexical memory networks [33].

The cholinergic system may also have a role in spreading activation. Specifically, patients with dementia who are taking acetylcholinesterase inhibitors exhibit reduced spreading activation in lexical memory networks as compared to dementia patients who were not taking these medications [34]. Research has indicated that dopamine and acetylcholine are asymmetrically distributed in the brain. Kononenko [66] reported greater cholinesterase activity in the left hemisphere, and Jayasundar [67] has reported a greater concentration of choline in left occipital and temporal regions. Others have found greater choline acetyltransferase activity in the left hemisphere [68], and particularly in the left first temporal gyrus [69]. Glick et al. [70] not only found that acetylcholine is predominantly in the left hemisphere but also that greater concentrations of dopamine were found in the left caudate and left globus pallidus. The asymmetrical distribution of dopamine and acetylcholine to the left hemisphere is consistent with their potential role in modulating spreading activation in verbal memory networks. However, given that design frequency may be related to spreading activation within the nonverbal memory networks of the right hemisphere, these same neurotransmitters may have less of a role in modulating nonverbal spreading activation.

Serotonin and norepinephrine, in contrast to dopamine and acetylcholine, seem to be asymmetrically distributed in the right hemisphere. Some have reported greater concentrations of norepinephrine in the right thalamic hemisphere [71]. Regarding serotonin, research has indicated that the right hemisphere has more serotonin metabolite (5HIAA) than the left hemisphere in the medial frontal region [72]. Further, Fink et al. [73] found significantly higher 5-HT1A receptor binding in the superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyri of the right hemisphere. Hence, the possibility exists that serotonin and norepinephrine may have more of a role in modulating spreading activation in nonverbal memory networks. Future research will need to be conducted to examine this possibility.

Limitations of the project include the relative sensitivity of the measures employed to assess left and right cerebral systems via double dissociation technique [74].

References

  1. Collins AM, Loftus EF (1975) A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychol Rev 82:407–428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Hebb DO (1949) The organization of behaviour. Wiley, New York. ISBN 978-0-471-36727-7

  3. Hebb DO (1955) Drives and the CNS. Psychol Rev 62:243–254

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Hebb DO (1959) A neuropsychological theory. In: Koch S (ed) Psychology: a study of science, vol 1., Sensory, Perceptual, and Physiological FormulationsMcGraw-Hill Book Company, New York, pp 622–643

    Google Scholar 

  5. McNamara TP (2005) Semantic priming: perspectives from memory and word recognition. Psychology Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  6. Jurden FH, Franzen MD, Callahan T, Ledbetter M (1996) Factoral equivalence of the Wechsler Memory Scale: revised across standardization and clinical samples. Appl Neuropsychol 3:65–74

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Moore PM, Baker GA (1997) Psychometric properties and factor structure of the Wechsler Memory Scale—revised in a sample of persons with intractable epilepsy. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 19:897–905

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Clark MS, Dennerstein L, Elkadi S, Guthrie JR, Bowden SC, Henderson VW (2004) Normative verbal and non-verbal memory test scores for Australian women aged 56-67. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 38:532–540

    Google Scholar 

  9. Heilbronner RL, Buck P, Adams RL (1989) Factor analysis of verbal and nonverbal clinical memory tests. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 4:299–309

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Larrabee GJ, Trahan DE, Curtiss G (1992) Construct validity of the Continuous Visual Memory Test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 7:395–405

    Google Scholar 

  11. Robinette RL, Sherer M, Adams RL (1993) The development of verbal and nonverbal factorially derived memory measures. J Clin Psychol 49:89–94

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Harrison DW (2015) Brain asymmetry and neural systems: Foundations in clinical neuroscience and neuropsychology. Springer, Berlin

    Book  Google Scholar 

  13. Baxendale SA, van Paesschen W, Thompson PJ, Connelly A, Duncan JS, Harkness WF, Shorvon SD (1998) The relationship between quantitative MRI and neuropsychological functioning in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 39:158–166

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Coleshill SG, Binnie CD, Morris RG, Alarcon G, van Emde Boas W, Velis DN, Simmons A, Polkey CE, van Veelen CWM, van Rijen PC (2004) Material-specific recognition memory deficits elicited by unilateral hippocampal electrical stimulation. J Neurosci 24:1612–1616

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Powell HWR, Koepp MJ, Symms PA, Boulby A, Salek-Haddadi PJ, Thompson JS, Duncan JS, Richardson MP (2005) Material-specific lateralization of memory encoding in the medial temporal lobe: blocked versus event-related design. NeuroImage 27:231–239

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Bohbot VD, Kalina M, Stepankova K, Spackova N, Petrides M, Nadel L (1998) Spatial memory deficits in patients with lesions to the right hippocampus and to the right parahippocampal cortex. Neuropsychologia 36:1217–1238

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Bornstein RA, Pakalnis A, Drake ME (1988) Verbal and nonverbal memory and learning in patients with complex partial seizures of temporal lobe origin. J Epilepsy 1:203–208

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Jones-Gotman M, Brulot M, McMackin D, Cendes F, Andermann F, Olivier A, Evans A, Peters T (1993) Word and design list learning deficits related to side of hippocampal atrophy as assessed by volumetric MRI measurements. Epilepsia 34:71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Kim H, Yi S, Son EI, Kim J (2003) Material-specific memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: effects of seizure laterality and language dominance. Neuropsychology 17:59–68

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Moscovitch DA, McAndrews MP (2002) Material-specific deficits in “remembering” in patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy and excisions. Neuropsychologia 40:1335–1342

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Graydon FJ, Nunn JA, Polkey CE, Morris RG (2001) Neuropsychological outcome and the extent of resection in the unilateral temporal lobectomy. Epilepsy Behav 2:140–151

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Pillon B, Bazin B, Deweer B, Ehrle N, Baulac M, Dubois B (1999) Specificity of memory deficits after right or left temporal lobectomy. Cortex 35:561–571

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Schacter DL, Cooper LA, Valdiserri M (1992) Implicit and explicit memory for novel visual objects in older and younger adults. Psychol Aging 7:299–308

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Cooper LA, Schacter DL, Ballesteros S, Moore C (1992) Priming and recognition of transformed three-dimensional objects: effects of size and reflection. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 18:43–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Schacter DL, Tharan M, Cooper LA, Rubens AB (1991) Preserved priming of novel objects in patients with memory disorders. J Cogn Neurosci 3:117–130

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Cooper LA, Schacter DL (1992) Dissociations between structural and episodic representations of visual objects. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 1:141–146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Allen PA, McNeal M, Kvak D (1992) Perhaps the lexicon is coded as a function of word frequency. J Mem Lang 31:826–844

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Allen PA, Smith AF, Lien MC, Weber TA, Madden DJ (1994) Word frequency effects at brief exposure durations: comment on Paap and Johansen. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23:1792–1797

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Allen PA, Wallace B, Weber TA (1995) Influence of case type, word frequency, and exposure duration on visual word recognition. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:914–934

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Foster PS, Yung RC, Branch KK, Stringer K, Ferguson BJ, Sullivan W, Drago V (2011) Increased spreading activation in depression. Brain Cogn 77:265–270

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Foster PS, Drago V, Yung RC, Pearson J, Stringer K, Giovannetti T, Libon D, Heilman KM (2013) Differential lexical and semantic spreading activation in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Alzheimer’s Dis Other Dement 28:501–507

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Foster PS, Roosa KM, Drago V, Branch K, Finney G, Heilman KM (2013) Recall of word lists is enhanced with increased spreading activation. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 20:553–566

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Foster PS, Drago V, FitzGerald DB, Skoblar BM, Crucian GP, Heilman KM (2008) Spreading activation of lexical-semantic networks in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 46:1908–1914

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Foster PS, Branch KK, Witt JC, Giovannetti T, Libon D, Heilman KM, Drago V (2012) Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors reduce spreading activation in dementia. Neuropsychologia 50:2093–2099

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Ruff RM (1996) Ruff figural fluency test. Psychological Assessment Resources Inc, Odessa

    Google Scholar 

  36. Ruff RM, Allen CC, Farrow CE, Niemann H, Wylie T (1994) Figural fluency: differential impairment in patients with left versus right frontal lobe lesions. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 9:41–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Foster PS, Williamson JB, Harrison DW (2005) The Ruff Figural Fluency Test: heightened right frontal lobe delta activity as a function of performance. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 20:427–434

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Hermann BP, Gold J, Pusakulich R, Wyler AR, Randolph C, Rankin G, Hoy W (1995) Wechsler adult intelligence scale—revised in the evaluation of anterior temporal lobectomy candidates. Epilepsia 36:480–487

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Dobbins C, Russell EW (1990) Left temporal lobe brain damage pattern on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. J Clin Psychol 46:863–868

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Zipf-Williams EM, Shear PK, Strongin D, Winegarden BJ, Morrell MJ (2000) Qualitative block design performance in epilepsy patients. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 15:149–157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Warrington EK, James M, Maciejewski C (1986) The WAIS as a lateralizing and localizing diagnostic instrument: a study of 656 patients with unilateral cerebral lesions. Neuropsychologia 24:223–239

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Allison JD, Meador KJ, Loring DW, Figueroa RE, Wright JC (2000) Functional MRI cerebral activation and deactivation during finger movement. Neurology 54:135–142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Harrison DW (1991) Concurrent verbal interference of right and left proximal and distal upper extremity tapping. Acta Psychol 76(2):121–132

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Ruff RM, Light RH, Evans RW (1987) The Ruff Figural Fluency Test: a normative study with adults. Dev Neuropsychol 3:37–51

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK (1996) Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX, pp 78204–2498

  46. Francis W, Kucera H (1982) Frequency analysis of English usage. Houghton Mifflin, New York

    Google Scholar 

  47. Banks SJ, Sziklas V, Sodums DJ, Jones-Gotman M (2012) fMRI of verbal and nonverbal memory processes in healthy and epileptogenic medial temporal lobes. Epilepsy Behav 25:42–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Golby AJ, Poldrack RA, Brewer JB, Spencer D, Desmond JE, Aron AP, Gabrieli JD (2001) Material-specific lateralization in the medial temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex during memory encoding. Brain 124:1841–1854

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Kelley WM, Miezin FM, McDermott KB, Buckner RL, Raichle ME, Cohen NJ, Ollinger JM, Akbudak E, Conturo TE, Snyder AZ, Petersen SE (1998) Hemispheric specialization in human dorsal frontal cortex and medial temporal lobe for verbal and nonverbal memory encoding. Neuron 20:927–936

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Edelstyn NM, Mayes AR, Denby C, Ellis SJ (2012) Impairment in material-specific long-term memory following unilateral mediodorsal thalamic damage and presumed partial disconnection of the mammillo-thalamic tract. J Neuropsychol 6:119–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Harrison DW (2015b) Thalamic and Hypothalamic Syndromes. In: Brain asymmetry and neural systems. Springer, Berlin, pp 169–180

  52. Tucker DM (1981) Lateral brain function, emotion, and conceptualization. Psychol Bull 89:19–46

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Bloom JS, Hynd GW (2005) The role of the corpus callosum in interhemispheric transfer of information: excitation or inhibition? Neuropsychol Rev 15:59–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Gourovitch ML, Kirkby BS, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR, Gold JM, Esposito G, Van Horn JD, Berman KF (2000) A comparison of rCBF patterns during letter and semantic fluency. Neuropsychology 14:353–360

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Henry JD, Crawford JR (2004) A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance in patients with traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology 18:621–628

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Demaree HA, Everhart DE, Youngstrom EA, Harrison DW (2005) Brain lateralization of emotional processing: historical roots and a future incorporating “dominance”. Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews 4(1):3–20

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Baldo JV, Shimamura AP, Delis DC, Kramer J, Kaplan E (2001) Verbal and design fluency in patients with frontal lobe lesions. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 7:586–596

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Herrmann MJ, Ehlis AC, Fallgatter AJ (2003) Frontal activation during a verbal-fluency task as measured by near-infrared spectroscopy. Brain Res Bull 30:51–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Stuss DT, Alexander MP, Hamer L, Palumbo C, Dempster R, Binns M, Levine B, Izukawa D (1998) The effects of focal anterior and posterior brain lesions on verbal fluency. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 4:265–278

    Google Scholar 

  60. Wilhite SC, Payne DE (1992) Learning and memory: the basis of behavior. Allyn and Bacon, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  61. Meyers JE, Meyers KR (1995) Rey complex figure and recognition trial: professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources Inc, Lutz

    Google Scholar 

  62. Benedict RHB (1997) Brief visuospatial memory test: revised: professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources Inc, Lutz

    Google Scholar 

  63. Cepeda C, Levine MS (1998) Dopamine and N-methyl-D-asparate receptor interactions in the neostriatum. Dev Neurosci 20:1–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Servan-Schreiber D, Printz H, Cohen JD (1990) A network model of catecholamine effects: gain, signal-to-noise ratio, and behavior. Science 49:892–895

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Kischka U, Kammer TH, Maier S, Weisbrod M, Thimm M, Spitzer M (1996) Dopaminergic modulation of semantic network activation. Neuropsychologia 34:1107–1113

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Kononenko VS (1981) Cholinesterase activity of nerve tissue as an indicator of asymmetry of brain centers. Hum Physiol 6:194–199

    Google Scholar 

  67. Jayasundar R (2002) Human brain: biochemical lateralization in normal subjects. Neurol India 50:267–271

    Google Scholar 

  68. Amaducci L, Sorbi S, Albanese A, Gainotti G (1981) Choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) activity differs in right and left human temporal lobes. Neurology 31:799–805

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Bracco L, Tiezzi A, Ginanneschi A, Campanella C, Amaducci L (1984) Lateralization of choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) activity in fetus and adult human brain. Neurosci Lett 50:301–305

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Glick SD, Ross DA, Hough LB (1982) Lateral asymmetry of neurotransmitters in human brain. Brain Res 234:53–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Oke A, Keller R, Mefford I, Adams RN (1978) Lateralization of norepinephrine in human thalamus. Science 200:1411–1413

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Arato M, Frecska E, Maccrimmon DJ, Guscott R, Saxena B, Tekes K, Tothfalusi L (1991) Serotonergic interhemispheric asymmetry: neurochemical and pharmaco-EEG evidence. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 15:759–764

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Fink M, Wadsak W, Savli M, Stein P, Moser U, Hahn A, Mien LK, Kletter K, Mitterhauser M, Kasper S, Lanzenberger R (2009) Lateralization of the serotonin-1A receptor distribution in language areas revealed by PET. NeuroImage 45:598–605

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Giovagnoli AR (1999) Verbal semantic memory in temporal lobe epilepsy. Acta Neurol Scand 99:334–339

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David W. Harrison.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Foster, P.S., Wakefield, C., Pryjmak, S. et al. Spreading activation in nonverbal memory networks. Brain Inf. 4, 187–199 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40708-016-0058-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40708-016-0058-y

Keywords